• Complain

V. S. Ramachandran - Phantoms in the Brain: Probing the Mysteries of the Human Mind

Here you can read online V. S. Ramachandran - Phantoms in the Brain: Probing the Mysteries of the Human Mind full text of the book (entire story) in english for free. Download pdf and epub, get meaning, cover and reviews about this ebook. year: 1998, publisher: William Morrow and Company, Inc., genre: Romance novel. Description of the work, (preface) as well as reviews are available. Best literature library LitArk.com created for fans of good reading and offers a wide selection of genres:

Romance novel Science fiction Adventure Detective Science History Home and family Prose Art Politics Computer Non-fiction Religion Business Children Humor

Choose a favorite category and find really read worthwhile books. Enjoy immersion in the world of imagination, feel the emotions of the characters or learn something new for yourself, make an fascinating discovery.

V. S. Ramachandran Phantoms in the Brain: Probing the Mysteries of the Human Mind

Phantoms in the Brain: Probing the Mysteries of the Human Mind: summary, description and annotation

We offer to read an annotation, description, summary or preface (depends on what the author of the book "Phantoms in the Brain: Probing the Mysteries of the Human Mind" wrote himself). If you haven't found the necessary information about the book — write in the comments, we will try to find it.

Neuroscientist V.S. Ramachandran is internationally renowned for uncovering answers to the deep and quirky questions of human nature that few scientists have dared to address. His bold insights about the brain are matched only by the stunning simplicity of his experiments -- using such low-tech tools as cotton swabs, glasses of water and dime-store mirrors. In Phantoms in the Brain, Dr. Ramachandranrecounts how his work with patients who have bizarre neurological disorders has shed new light on the deep architecture of the brain, and what these findings tell us about who we are, how we construct our body image, why we laugh or become depressed, why we may believe in God, how we make decisions, deceive ourselves and dream, perhaps even why were so clever at philosophy, music and art. Some of his most notable cases:A woman paralyzed on the left side of her body who believes she is lifting a tray of drinks with both hands offers a unique opportunity to test Freuds theory of denial. A man who insists he is talking with God challenges us to ask: Could we be wired for religious experience? A woman who hallucinates cartoon characters illustrates how, in a sense, we are all hallucinating, all the time.Dr. Ramachandrans inspired medical detective work pushes the boundaries of medicines last great frontier -- the human mind -- yielding new and provocative insights into the big questions about consciousness and the self.

V. S. Ramachandran: author's other books


Who wrote Phantoms in the Brain: Probing the Mysteries of the Human Mind? Find out the surname, the name of the author of the book and a list of all author's works by series.

Phantoms in the Brain: Probing the Mysteries of the Human Mind — read online for free the complete book (whole text) full work

Below is the text of the book, divided by pages. System saving the place of the last page read, allows you to conveniently read the book "Phantoms in the Brain: Probing the Mysteries of the Human Mind" online for free, without having to search again every time where you left off. Put a bookmark, and you can go to the page where you finished reading at any time.

Light

Font size:

Reset

Interval:

Bookmark:

Make
PHANTOMS IN THE BRAIN Probing the Mysteries of the Human Mind VS - photo 1

PHANTOMS

IN THE

BRAIN

Probing the Mysteries of the Human Mind

V.S. Ramachandran, M.D., Ph.D., and Sandra Blakeslee

Copyright 1998

ISBN 0688152473

To my mother, Meenakshi

To my father, Subramanian

To my brother, Ravi

To Diane, Mani and Jayakrishna

To all my former teachers in India and England

To Saraswathy, the goddess of learning, music and wisdom

Foreword

The great neurologists and psychiatrists of the nineteenth and early twentieth centuries were masters of description, and some of their case histories provided an almost novelistic richness of detail. Silas Weir Mitchellwho was a novelist as well as a neurologistprovided unforgettable descriptions of the phantom limbs (or "sensory ghosts," as he first called them) in soldiers who had been injured on the battlefields of the Civil War. Joseph Babinski, the great French neurologist, described an even more extraordinary syndromeanosognosia, the inability to perceive that one side of one's own body is paralyzed and the oftenbizarre attribution of the paralyzed side to another person. (Such a patient might say of his or her own left side, "It's my brother's" or "It's yours.") Dr. V.S. Ramachandran, one of the most interesting neuroscientists of our time, has done seminal work on the nature and treatment of phantom limbsthose obdurate and sometimes tormenting ghosts of arms and legs lost years or decades before but not forgotten by the brain. A phantom may at first feel like a normal limb, a part of the normal body image; but, cut off from normal sensation or action, it may assume a pathological character, becoming intrusive, "paralyzed," deformed, or excruciatingly painfulphantom fingers may dig into a phantom palm with an unspeakable, unstoppable intensity. The fact that the pain and the phantom are

"unreal" is of no help, and may indeed make them more difficult to treat, for one may be unable to unclench the seemingly paralyzed phantom. In an attempt to alleviate such phantoms, physicians and their patients have been driven to extreme and desperate measures: making the amputation stump shorter and shorter, cutting pain or sensory tracts in the spinal cord, destroying pain centers in the brain itself. But all too frequently, none of these work; the phantom, and the phantom pain, almost invariably return.

To these seemingly intractable problems, Ramachandran brings a fresh and different approach, which stems from his inquiries as to what phantoms are, and how and where they are generated in the nervous system. It has been classically considered that representations in the brain, including those of body image and phantoms, are fixed. But Ramachandran (and now others) has shown that striking reorganizations in body image occur very rapidlywithin fortyeight hours, and possibly much less following the amputation of a limb.

Phantoms, in his view, are

generated by such reorganizations of body image in the sensory cortex and may then be maintained by what he terms a "learned" paralysis. But if there are such rapid changes underlying the genesis of a phantom, if there is such plasticity in the cortex, can the process be reversed? Can the brain be tricked into unlearning a phantom?

By using an ingenious "virtual reality" device, a simple box with a transposing mirror, Ramachandran has found that a patient may be helped by merely being given the sight of a normal limbthe patient's own normal right arm, for example, now seen on the left side of the body, in place of the phantom. The result of this may be instantaneous and magical: The normal look of the arm competes with the feel of the phantom.

The first effect of this is that a deformed phantom may straighten out, a paralyzed phantom may move; eventually, there may be no more phantom at all. Ramachandran speaks here, with characteristic humor, of

"the first successful amputation of a phantom limb," and of how, if the phantom is extinguished, its pain must also gofor if there is nothing to embody it, then it can no longer survive. (Mrs. Gradgrind, in Hard Times, asked if she had a pain, replied, "There is a pain somewhere in the room, but I cannot be sure that I have got it." But this was her confusion, or Dickens's joke, for one cannot have a pain except in oneself.) Can equally simple "tricks" assist patients with anosognosia, patients who cannot recognize one of their sides as their own? Here too, Ramachandran finds, mirrors may be of great use in enabling such patients to reclaim the previously denied side as their own; though in other patients, the loss of "leftness," the bisection of one's 2

body and world, is so profound that mirrors may induce an even deeper, throughthelookingglass confusion, a groping to see if there is not someone lurking "behind" or "in" the mirror. (Ramachandran is the first to describe this "mirror agnosia.") It is a measure not only of Ramachandran's tenacity of mind but of his delicate and supportive relationship with patients that he has been able to pursue these syndromes to their depths.

The deeply strange business of mirror agnosia, and that of misattributing one's own limbs to others, are often dismissed by physicians as irrational. But these problems are also considered carefully by Ramachandran, who sees them not as groundless or crazy, but as emergency defense measures constructed by the unconscious to deal with sudden overwhelming bewilderments about one's body and the space around it. They are, he feels, quite normal defense mechanisms (denial, repression, projection, confabulation, and so on) such as Freud delineated as uni

versai strategies of the unconscious when forced to accommodate the intolerable or unintelligible. Such an understanding removes such patients from the realm of the mad or freakish and restores them to the realm of discourse and reasonalbeit the discourse and reason of the unconscious.

Another syndrome of misidentification that Ramachandran considers is Capgras' syndrome, where the patient sees familiar and loved figures as impostors. Here too, he is able to delineate a clear neurological basis for the syndromethe removal of the usual and crucial affective cues to recognition, coupled with a not unnatural interpretation of the now affectless perceptions ("He can't be my father, because I feel nothinghe must be a sort of simulacrum").

Dr. Ramachandran has countless other interests too: in the nature of religious experience and the remarkable

"mystical" syndromes associated with dysfunction in the temporal lobes, in the neurology of laughter and tickling, anda vast realmin the neurology of suggestion and placebos. Like the perceptual psychologist Richard Gregory (with whom he has published fascinating work on a range of subjects, from the fillingin of the blind spot to visual illusions and protective colorations), Ramachandran has a flair for seeing what is fundamentally important and is prepared to turn his hand, his freshness, his inventiveness, to almost anything.

All of these subjects, in his hands, become windows into the way our nervous systems, our worlds, and our very selves are constituted, so that his work becomes, as he likes to say, a form of "experimental epistemology." He is, in this way, a natural philosopher in the eighteenthcentury sense, though with all the knowledge and knowhow of the late twentieth century behind him.

In his Preface, Ramachandran tells us of the nineteenthcentury science books he especially enjoyed as a boy: Michael Faraday's Chemical History of a Candle, works by Charles Darwin, Humphry Davy and Thomas Huxley. There was no distinction at this time between academic and popular writing, but rather the notion that one could be deep and serious but completely accessible, all at once. Later, Ramachandran tells us, he enjoyed the books of George Gamow, Lewis Thomas, Peter Medawar, and then Carl Sagan and Stephen Jay Gould.

Next page
Light

Font size:

Reset

Interval:

Bookmark:

Make

Similar books «Phantoms in the Brain: Probing the Mysteries of the Human Mind»

Look at similar books to Phantoms in the Brain: Probing the Mysteries of the Human Mind. We have selected literature similar in name and meaning in the hope of providing readers with more options to find new, interesting, not yet read works.


Reviews about «Phantoms in the Brain: Probing the Mysteries of the Human Mind»

Discussion, reviews of the book Phantoms in the Brain: Probing the Mysteries of the Human Mind and just readers' own opinions. Leave your comments, write what you think about the work, its meaning or the main characters. Specify what exactly you liked and what you didn't like, and why you think so.