• Complain

Patricia S. Churchland - Braintrust: What Neuroscience Tells Us about Morality

Here you can read online Patricia S. Churchland - Braintrust: What Neuroscience Tells Us about Morality full text of the book (entire story) in english for free. Download pdf and epub, get meaning, cover and reviews about this ebook. year: 2011, publisher: Princeton University Press, genre: Romance novel. Description of the work, (preface) as well as reviews are available. Best literature library LitArk.com created for fans of good reading and offers a wide selection of genres:

Romance novel Science fiction Adventure Detective Science History Home and family Prose Art Politics Computer Non-fiction Religion Business Children Humor

Choose a favorite category and find really read worthwhile books. Enjoy immersion in the world of imagination, feel the emotions of the characters or learn something new for yourself, make an fascinating discovery.

No cover
  • Book:
    Braintrust: What Neuroscience Tells Us about Morality
  • Author:
  • Publisher:
    Princeton University Press
  • Genre:
  • Year:
    2011
  • Rating:
    3 / 5
  • Favourites:
    Add to favourites
  • Your mark:
    • 60
    • 1
    • 2
    • 3
    • 4
    • 5

Braintrust: What Neuroscience Tells Us about Morality: summary, description and annotation

We offer to read an annotation, description, summary or preface (depends on what the author of the book "Braintrust: What Neuroscience Tells Us about Morality" wrote himself). If you haven't found the necessary information about the book — write in the comments, we will try to find it.

What is morality? Where does it come from? And why do most of us heed its call most of the time? In Braintrust, neurophilosophy pioneer Patricia Churchland argues that morality originates in the biology of the brain. She describes the neurobiological platform of bonding that, modified by evolutionary pressures and cultural values, has led to human styles of moral behavior. The result is a provocative genealogy of morals that asks us to reevaluate the priority given to religion, absolute rules, and pure reason in accounting for the basis of morality. Moral values, Churchland argues, are rooted in a behavior common to all mammals--the caring for offspring. The evolved structure, processes, and chemistry of the brain incline humans to strive not only for self-preservation but for the well-being of allied selves--first offspring, then mates, kin, and so on, in wider and wider caring circles. Separation and exclusion cause pain, and the company of loved ones causes pleasure; responding to feelings of social pain and pleasure, brains adjust their circuitry to local customs. In this way, caring is apportioned, conscience molded, and moral intuitions instilled. A key part of the story is oxytocin, an ancient body-and-brain molecule that, by decreasing the stress response, allows humans to develop the trust in one another necessary for the development of close-knit ties, social institutions, and morality. A major new account of what really makes us moral, Braintrust challenges us to reconsider the origins of some of our most cherished values.Table of contents : Patricia S. Churchland - Braintrust......Page 1Contents......Page 8List of Illustrations......Page 101. Introduction......Page 142. Brain-Based Values......Page 25But Surely Only Humans Are Moral?......Page 363. Caring and Caring For......Page 40Family Values: Belonging and Wanting to Belong......Page 46Mate Attachment......Page 59The Mechanisms of Mate Attachment......Page 61What Else besides Oxytocin?......Page 67Male Parenting......Page 69What Is the Connection between Attachment and Morality?......Page 724. Cooperating and Trusting......Page 76What Exactly Is Cooperation in Mammals?......Page 78Cooperation in Mammals: A Few Examples......Page 81Trust and Oxytocin: What Do We Know about Its Effects on Humans?......Page 84Punishment and Cooperativity......Page 94The Effect of Social Tension on Cooperativity......Page 99Evolution and Human Cooperation......Page 1025. Networking: Genes, Brains, and Behavior......Page 108Genetic Networks......Page 110Innate Moral Principles and Innate Moral Foundations......Page 116Jonathan Haidt and Moral Foundations......Page 1256. Skills for a Social Life......Page 131Social Knowledge, Social Learning, Social Decision-Making......Page 140Acquiring a Conscience......Page 143Attributing Mental States to Self and Others......Page 145Mirror Neurons and Mental Attribution (Theory of Mind)......Page 148Humans, Intentions, and Mirror Neurons......Page 158Mirroring and Empathy......Page 160Imitation and Mirror Neurons......Page 166Theory of Mind, Autism, and Mirror Neurons......Page 167Imitation, Unconscious Mimicry, and Social Capacities......Page 1697. Not as a Rule......Page 176Kant and His Categorical Imperative......Page 186Consequentialism and Maximizing Utility......Page 188Facts about Rule Use......Page 194Normativity and the Moral Ought......Page 198The Naturalistic Fallacy......Page 199Conscience and Morality......Page 204Morality and Religion......Page 207Does This Mean Morality Is an Illusion?......Page 212Morality, Trust, and Cultural Niche Construction......Page 2141. Introduction......Page 2182. Brain-Based Values......Page 2203. Caring and Caring For......Page 2224. Cooperating and Trusting......Page 2285. Networking......Page 2326. Skills for a Social Life......Page 2367. Not as a Rule......Page 2438. Religion and Morality......Page 246Bibliography......Page 248Acknowledgments......Page 272Index......Page 274

Patricia S. Churchland: author's other books


Who wrote Braintrust: What Neuroscience Tells Us about Morality? Find out the surname, the name of the author of the book and a list of all author's works by series.

Braintrust: What Neuroscience Tells Us about Morality — read online for free the complete book (whole text) full work

Below is the text of the book, divided by pages. System saving the place of the last page read, allows you to conveniently read the book "Braintrust: What Neuroscience Tells Us about Morality" online for free, without having to search again every time where you left off. Put a bookmark, and you can go to the page where you finished reading at any time.

Light

Font size:

Reset

Interval:

Bookmark:

Make
braintrust braintrust What Neuroscience Tells Us about Morality Patricia S - photo 1

braintrust

braintrust

What Neuroscience Tells Us about Morality

Patricia S. Churchland

Princeton University Press Princeton and Oxford

Copyright 2011 by Princeton University Press

Published by Princeton University Press, 41 William Street,
Princeton, New Jersey 08540
In the United Kingdom: Princeton University Press, 6 Oxford Street,
Woodstock, Oxfordshire OX20 1TW

press.princeton.edu

All Rights Reserved

Jacket illustration: Brain Sebastian Kaulitzki/Shutterstock

LIBRARY OF CONGRESS CATALOGING-IN-PUBLICATION DATA
Churchland, Patricia Smith.
Braintrust : what neuroscience tells us about morality / Patricia S.
Churchland.

p. ; cm.
What neuroscience tells us about morality
Includes bibliographical references and index.
ISBN 978-0-691-13703-2 (hardcover : alk. paper)
1. Ethics. 2. Neurobiology.
I. Title. II. Title: What neuroscience tells us about morality.
[DNLM: 1. Neuropsychology. 2. Morals. 3. Neurosciencesmethods.
4. Philosophy. 5. Social Behavior. WL 103.5]
QP430.C58 2011
612.8dc22 2010043584

British Library Cataloging-in-Publication Data is available

This book has been composed in Electra

Printed on acid-free paper.

Printed in the United States of America

1 3 5 7 9 10 8 6 4 2

Its a vice to trust everyone, and equally a vice to trust no one.

Seneca

This is our mammalian conflict: what to give to others and what to keep for yourself. Treading that line, keeping others in check and being kept in check by them, is what we call morality.

Ian McEwan, Eternal Love

Contents
Illustrations

braintrust

Introduction

Trial by ordeal seemed to me, as I learned about it in school, ridiculously unfair. How could it have endured as an institution in Europe for hundreds of years? The central idea was simple: with Gods intervention, innocence would plainly reveal itself, as the accused thief sank to the bottom of the pond, or the accused adulterer remained unburned by the red hot poker placed in his hand. Only the guilty would drown or burn. (For witches, the ordeal was less forgiving: if the accused witch drowned she was presumed innocent; if she bobbed to the surface, she was guilty, whereupon she was hauled off to a waiting fire.) With time on our hands, my friend and I concocted a plan. She would falsely accuse me of stealing her purse, and then I would lay my hand on the stove and see whether it burned. We fully expected it would burn, and it did. So if the test was that obvious, how could people have trusted to trial by ordeal as a system of justice?

From the medieval clerics, the answer would have been that our test was frivolous, and that God would not deign to intervene with a miracle for the benefit of kids fooling around. That answer seemed to us a bit cooked up. What is the evidence God ever intervened on behalf of the wrongly accused? A further difficulty concerned nonbelievers, such as those not yet reached by missionaries, or... maybe me? Still, this answer alerted us to the matter of metaphysical (or as we said then, otherworldly) beliefs in moral practices, along with the realization that what seemed to us obvious about fairness in determining guilt might not be obvious after all.

My history teacher tried to put the medieval practice in context, aiming to soften slightly our sense of superiority over our medieval ancestors: in trial by ordeal, the guilty were more likely to confess, since they believed God would not intervene on their behalf, whereas the innocent, convinced that God would help out, were prepared to go to trial. So the system might work pretty well for getting confessions from the guilty, even if it did poorly for protecting the innocent. This answer alerted us to the presence of pragmatics in moral practices, which struck us as a little less lofty than we had been led to expect. How hideously unfair if you were innocent and did go to trial. I could visualize myself, bound by ropes, drowning in a river after being accused of witchcraft by my piano teacher.

So what is it to be fair? How do we know what to count as fair? Why do we regard trial by ordeal as wrong? Thus opens the door into the vast tangled forest of questions about right and wrong, good and evil, virtues and vices. For most of my adult life as a philosopher, I shied away from plunging unreservedly into these sorts of questions about morality. This was largely because I could not see a systematic way through that tangled forest, and because a lot of contemporary moral philosophy, though venerated in academic halls, was completely untethered to the hard and fast; that is, it had no strong connection to evolution or to the brain, and hence was in peril of floating on a sea of mere, albeit confident, opinion. And no doubt the medieval clerics were every bit as confident.

It did seem that likely Aristotle, Hume, and Darwin were right: we are social by nature. But what does that actually mean in terms of our brains and our genes? To make progress beyond the broad hunches about our nature, we need something solid to attach the claim to. Without relevant, real data from evolutionary biology, neuroscience, and genetics, I could not see how to tether ideas about our nature to the hard and fast.

Despite being flummoxed, I began to appreciate that recent developments in the biological sciences allow us to see through the tangle, to begin to discern pathways revealed by new data. The phenomenon of moral values, hitherto so puzzling, is now less so. Not entirely clear, just less puzzling. By drawing on converging new data from neuroscience, evolutionary biology, experimental psychology, and genetics, and given a philosophical framework consilient with those data, we can now meaningfully approach the question of where values come from.

The wealth of data can easily swamp us, but the main story line can be set out in a fairly straightforward way. My aim here is to explain what is probably true about our social nature, and what that involves in terms of the neural platform for moral behavior. As will become plain, the platform is only the platform; it is not the whole story of human moral values. Social practices, and culture more generally, are not my focus here, although they are, of course, hugely important in the values people live by. Additionally, particular moral dilemmas, such as when a war is a just war, or whether inheritance taxes are fair, are not the focus here.

Although remarks of a general sort concerning our nature often fall on receptive ears, those same ears may become rather deaf when the details of brain circuitry begin to be discussed. When we speak of the possibility of linking large-scale questions about our mind with developments in the neurosciences, there are those who are wont to wag their fingers and warn us about the perils of scientism. That means, so far as I can tell, the offense of taking science into places where allegedly it has no business, of being in the grip of the grand delusion that science can explain everything, do everything. Scientism, as I have been duly wagged, is overreaching.

The complaint that a scientific approach to understanding morality commits the sin of scientism does really exaggerate what science is up to, since the scientific enterprise does not aim to displace the arts or the humanities. Shakespeare and Mozart and Caravaggio are not in competition with protein kinases and micro RNA. On the other hand, it is true that philosophical claims about the nature of things, such as moral intuition, are vulnerable. Here, philosophy and science are working the same ground, and evidence should trump armchair reflection. In the present case, the claim is not that science will wade in and tell us for every dilemma what

Next page
Light

Font size:

Reset

Interval:

Bookmark:

Make

Similar books «Braintrust: What Neuroscience Tells Us about Morality»

Look at similar books to Braintrust: What Neuroscience Tells Us about Morality. We have selected literature similar in name and meaning in the hope of providing readers with more options to find new, interesting, not yet read works.


Reviews about «Braintrust: What Neuroscience Tells Us about Morality»

Discussion, reviews of the book Braintrust: What Neuroscience Tells Us about Morality and just readers' own opinions. Leave your comments, write what you think about the work, its meaning or the main characters. Specify what exactly you liked and what you didn't like, and why you think so.