• Complain

Michael Tomasello - A Natural History of Human Thinking

Here you can read online Michael Tomasello - A Natural History of Human Thinking full text of the book (entire story) in english for free. Download pdf and epub, get meaning, cover and reviews about this ebook. year: 2014, genre: Romance novel. Description of the work, (preface) as well as reviews are available. Best literature library LitArk.com created for fans of good reading and offers a wide selection of genres:

Romance novel Science fiction Adventure Detective Science History Home and family Prose Art Politics Computer Non-fiction Religion Business Children Humor

Choose a favorite category and find really read worthwhile books. Enjoy immersion in the world of imagination, feel the emotions of the characters or learn something new for yourself, make an fascinating discovery.

No cover

A Natural History of Human Thinking: summary, description and annotation

We offer to read an annotation, description, summary or preface (depends on what the author of the book "A Natural History of Human Thinking" wrote himself). If you haven't found the necessary information about the book — write in the comments, we will try to find it.

Tool-making or culture, language or religious belief: ever since Darwin, thinkers have struggled to identify what fundamentally differentiates human beings from other animals. In this much-anticipated book, Michael Tomasello weaves his twenty years of comparative studies of humans and great apes into a compelling argument that cooperative social interaction is the key to our cognitive uniqueness. Once our ancestors learned to put their heads together with others to pursue shared goals, humankind was on an evolutionary path all its own.Tomasello argues that our prehuman ancestors, like todays great apes, were social beings who could solve problems by thinking. But they were almost entirely competitive, aiming only at their individual goals. As ecological changes forced them into more cooperative living arrangements, early humans had to coordinate their actions and communicate their thoughts with collaborative partners. Tomasellos shared intentionality hypothesis captures how these more socially complex forms of life led to more conceptually complex forms of thinking. In order to survive, humans had to learn to see the world from multiple social perspectives, to draw socially recursive inferences, and to monitor their own thinking via the normative standards of the group. Even language and culture arose from the preexisting need to work together. What differentiates us most from other great apes, Tomasello proposes, are the new forms of thinking engendered by our new forms of collaborative and communicative interaction.A Natural History of Human Thinking is the most detailed scientific analysis to date of the connection between human sociality and cognition.

Michael Tomasello: author's other books


Who wrote A Natural History of Human Thinking? Find out the surname, the name of the author of the book and a list of all author's works by series.

A Natural History of Human Thinking — read online for free the complete book (whole text) full work

Below is the text of the book, divided by pages. System saving the place of the last page read, allows you to conveniently read the book "A Natural History of Human Thinking" online for free, without having to search again every time where you left off. Put a bookmark, and you can go to the page where you finished reading at any time.

Light

Font size:

Reset

Interval:

Bookmark:

Make

A Natural History of Human Thinking

A Natural History of Human Thinking

A Natural History of Human Thinking - image 1

Michael Tomasello

Picture 2

Harvard University Press

Cambridge, Massachusetts

London, England

2014

Copyright 2014 by the President and Fellows of Harvard College

All rights reserved

Jacket design by Tim Jones.

The Library of Congress has cataloged the printed edition as follows:

Tomasello, Michael.

A natural history of human thinking / Michael Tomasello.

pages cm

Includes bibliographical references and index.

ISBN 978-0-674-72477-8 (hardcover : alk. paper)

1. CognitionSocial aspects. 2. Evolutionary psychology. 3. Psychology, Comparative. I. Title.

BF311.T6473 2014

153dc23 2013020185

For Rita, Anya, Leo, and Chiara

Contents

This book is a sequelor, better, a prequelto The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition (Harvard University Press, 1999). But it also has a slightly different focus. In the 1999 book the question was what makes human cognition unique, and the answer was culture. Individual human beings develop uniquely powerful cognitive skills because they grow to maturity in the midst of all kinds of cultural artifacts and practices, including a conventional language, and of course they have the cultural learning skills necessary to master them. Individuals internalize the artifacts and practices they encounter, and these then serve to mediate all of their cognitive interactions with the world.

In the current book, the question is similar: what makes human thinking unique? And the answer is similar as well: human thinking is fundamentally cooperative. But this slightly different question and slightly different answer lead to a very different book. The 1999 book was clean and simple because the data we had comparing apes and humans were so sparse. We could thus say things like Only humans understand others as intentional agents, and this enables human culture. But we now know that the picture is more complex than this. Great apes appear to know much more about others as intentional agents than previously believed, and still they do not have human-like culture or cognition. Based on much research reported here, the critical difference now seems to be that humans not only understand others as intentional agents but also put their heads together with others in acts of shared intentionality, including everything from concrete acts of collaborative problem solving to complex cultural institutions. The focus now is thus less on culture as a process of transmission and more on culture as a process of social coordinationand indeed, we argue here that modern human cultures were made possible by an earlier evolutionary step in which individuals made a living by coordinating with others in relatively simple acts of collaborative foraging.

The specific focus on thinking means that this book does not simply document that humans participate in shared intentionality in a way that their nearest primate relatives do not, which has been done elsewhere. Rather, in addition, it examines the underlying thinking processes involved. To describe the nature of these thinking processesin particular, to distinguish human thinking from that of other apeswe must characterize its component processes of cognitive representation, inference, and self-monitoring. The shared intentionality hypothesis claims that all three of these components were transformed in two key steps during human evolution. In both cases, the transformation was part of a larger change of social interaction and organization in which humans were forced to adopt more cooperative lifeways. In order to survive and thrive, humans were forced, twice, to find new ways to coordinate their behavior with others in collaborative (and then cultural) activities and to coordinate their intentional states with others in cooperative (and then conventional) communication. And this transformed, twice, the way that humans think.

The writing of this book, as most others, was made possible by the support of many institutions and people. I would like to thank the University of Pittsburgh Center for Philosophy of Science (John Norton, director and seminar leader extraordinaire) for hosting me for one peaceful semester of concentrated writing in the spring of 2012. I especially benefited during this stay from Bob Brandoms generosity with his time and thoughts on many topics central to the current enterprise. I thank Celia Brownell at the Pitt Department of Psychology and Andy Norman at Carnegie Mellon for many useful discussions during this semester as well. The ensuing summer I benefited greatly from presenting the themes of the book to the SIAS Summer Institute titled The Second Person: Comparative Perspectives , organized in Berlin by Jim Conant and Sebastian Rdl. The book is better for all of these encounters.

With regard to the manuscript itself, I would like to thank Larry Barsalou, Mattia Galloti, Henrike Moll, and Marco Schmidt for reading various chapters and providing very useful feedback. Of special importance, Richard Moore and Hannes Rakoczy each read the entire manuscript at a fairly early stage and provided a number of trenchant comments and suggestions, regarding both content and presentation. Thanks also to Elizabeth Knoll and three anonymous reviewers at Harvard University Press for a number of helpful comments and criticisms on the penultimate draft.

Last and most important, I thank my wife, Rita Svetlova, for providing constant and detailed critical commentary and suggestions throughout. Many ideas were made clearer through discussion with her, and confusing passages were made clear, or at least clearer, by her literate eye.

Only cooperation constitutes a process that can produce reason.

JEAN PIAGET, SOCIOLOGICAL STUDIES

Thinking would seem to be a completely solitary activity. And so it is for other animal species. But for humans, thinking is like a jazz musician improvising a novel riff in the privacy of his own room. It is a solitary activity all right, but on an instrument made by others for that general purpose, after years of playing with and learning from other practitioners, in a musical genre with a rich history of legendary riffs, for an imagined audience of jazz aficionados. Human thinking is individual improvisation enmeshed in a sociocultural matrix.

How did this novel form of socially infused thinking come to be, and how does it work? One set of classic theorists has emphasized the role of culture and its artifacts in making possible certain types of individual thinking. For example, Hegel (1807) argued that the social practices, institutions, and ideologies of a particular culture at a particular historical epoch constitute a necessary conceptual framework for individual human reason (see also Collingwood, 1946). Peirce (19311935) claimed more specifically that virtually all of humans most sophisticated types of thinking, including most especially mathematics and formal logic, are possible only because individuals have available to them culturally created symbolic artifacts such as Arabic numerals and logical notation. Vygotsky (1978) emphasized that human children grow up in the midst of the tools and symbols of their culture, including especially the linguistic symbols that preorganize their worlds for them, and during ontogeny they internalize the use of these artifacts, leading to the kind of internal dialogue that is one prototype of human thinking (see also Bakhtin, 1981).

The other set of classic theorists has focused on the fundamental processes of social coordination that make human culture and language possible in the first place. Mead (1934) pointed out that when humans interact with one another, especially in communication, they are able to imagine themselves in the role of the other and to take the others perspective on themselves. Piaget (1928) argued further that these role-taking and perspective-taking abilitiesalong with a cooperative attitudenot only make culture and language possible but also make possible reasoning in which individuals subordinate their own point of view to the normative standards of the group. And Wittgenstein (1955) explicated several different ways in which the appropriate use of a linguistic convention or cultural rule depends on a preexisting set of shared social practices and judgments (forms of life), which constitute the pragmatic infrastructure from which all uses of language and rules gain their interpersonal significance. These social infrastructure theorists, as we may call them, all share the belief that language and culture are only the icing on the cake of humans ultrasocial ways of relating to the world cognitively.

Next page
Light

Font size:

Reset

Interval:

Bookmark:

Make

Similar books «A Natural History of Human Thinking»

Look at similar books to A Natural History of Human Thinking. We have selected literature similar in name and meaning in the hope of providing readers with more options to find new, interesting, not yet read works.


Reviews about «A Natural History of Human Thinking»

Discussion, reviews of the book A Natural History of Human Thinking and just readers' own opinions. Leave your comments, write what you think about the work, its meaning or the main characters. Specify what exactly you liked and what you didn't like, and why you think so.