• Complain

John Bolender - The Self-Organizing Social Mind

Here you can read online John Bolender - The Self-Organizing Social Mind full text of the book (entire story) in english for free. Download pdf and epub, get meaning, cover and reviews about this ebook. year: 2010, publisher: A Bradford Book, genre: Romance novel. Description of the work, (preface) as well as reviews are available. Best literature library LitArk.com created for fans of good reading and offers a wide selection of genres:

Romance novel Science fiction Adventure Detective Science History Home and family Prose Art Politics Computer Non-fiction Religion Business Children Humor

Choose a favorite category and find really read worthwhile books. Enjoy immersion in the world of imagination, feel the emotions of the characters or learn something new for yourself, make an fascinating discovery.

John Bolender The Self-Organizing Social Mind

The Self-Organizing Social Mind: summary, description and annotation

We offer to read an annotation, description, summary or preface (depends on what the author of the book "The Self-Organizing Social Mind" wrote himself). If you haven't found the necessary information about the book — write in the comments, we will try to find it.

In The Self-Organizing Social Mind, John Bolender proposes a new
explanation for the forms of social relations. He argues that the core of social-relational
cognition exhibits beauty -- in the physicists sense of the word, associated with symmetry.
Bolender describes a fundamental set of patterns in interpersonal cognition, which account for the
resulting structures of social life in terms of their symmetries and the breaking of those
symmetries. He further describes the symmetries of the four fundamental social relations as ordered
in a nested series akin to what one finds in the formation of a snowflake or spiral galaxy. Symmetry
breaking organizes the neural activity generating the cognitive models that structure our social
relationships.

Bolenders primary claim is that there exists a social pattern
generator analogous to the central pattern generators associated with locomotion in many animal
species. Spontaneous symmetry breaking structures the activity of the social pattern generator just
as it does in central pattern generators.

Bolenders hypothesis that relational
cognition results from self-organization is entirely novel, distinct from other theories that
describe sociality in terms of evolution or environment. It presents a picture of social-relational
cognition as resembling something inorganic. In doing so it reveals deep connections among
cognition, biology, and the inorganic world. One can go too far, he acknowledges, in taking a solely
dynamical view of the mind; the minds innate functional complexity must be due to natural
selection. But this does not mean that every simple mental feature is the result of natural
selection. By noting a descending symmetry subgroup chain at the core of relational cognition,
Bolender takes the first step in an important investigation.

John Bolender: author's other books


Who wrote The Self-Organizing Social Mind? Find out the surname, the name of the author of the book and a list of all author's works by series.

The Self-Organizing Social Mind — read online for free the complete book (whole text) full work

Below is the text of the book, divided by pages. System saving the place of the last page read, allows you to conveniently read the book "The Self-Organizing Social Mind" online for free, without having to search again every time where you left off. Put a bookmark, and you can go to the page where you finished reading at any time.

Light

Font size:

Reset

Interval:

Bookmark:

Make
John Bolender - photo 1
Picture 2
Picture 3

John Bolender

Picture 4

Picture 5

Picture 6

Picture 7

vii

xiii

Picture 8

Thoughts about thoughts about thoughts about social relations ...

In this extraordinarily original book, John Bolender outlines not simply a new explanation of the forms of social relations, but an entirely new kind of explanation. Science is the process of looking for patterns, and Bolender describes a fundamental set of patterns-and patterns of patterns-in social life. He explains the fundamental structures of social life-and the systematic relations among them-in terms of their symmetries and the breaking of those symmetries. Electrons are symmetrical to positrons; matter is symmetrical to anti-matter. Symmetries structure crystals and galaxies, the movements of planets and the oscillations of waves, the shapes of flowers and seashells, the anatomy and the locomotion of animals. Bolender draws our attention to the symmetries of social relationships and observes that some relationships are more symmetrical than others. Moreover, the symmetries of some relationships are subsets of the symmetries of other relationships, such that when one of the symmetries of a relationship is broken, the remaining symmetries form another type of relationship. So the symmetries of the four fundamental types of social relationships are ordered in a nested series. In short, Bolender shows that symmetry breaking-a fundamental principle of physics-also structures the universe of social relationships-or, actually, the cognitive models for social relationships and, he plausibly argues, patterns of neural activity that generate the relational models.

When I set out relational models theory (Fiske 1991), I underlined their homology with the four basic types of measurement scales and suggested that their homogeneity and uniqueness under specific transformations also makes them especially well suited to coordinating social relations. I observed that the respective relational models formed a descending, nested series with successively fewer degrees of uniqueness. (For a bibliography of research on relational models, see www.rmt.ucla.edu.) Bolender points out that what measurement theorists call uniqueness under a transformation is what physicists call a symmetry, and that the four fundamental relational models are linked to each other as a chain of descending symmetry subgroups-the property of structures that result from spontaneous symmetry breakdown. Thus he brings sociality into the realm of brain physics.

This chain of symmetries explanation is entirely distinct from theories of sociality based on natural selection, behavioral ecology, genetics, neuroanatomy, neurochemistry, learning or socialization, ontogeny, culture, economy, or communication. In a certain respect, it is a deeply cognitive account, but it takes an entirely novel approach to cognition. Great scientific advances are based on conceiving entirely new approaches to basic phenomena, often by applying perspectives from fields previously thought to be distant to the phenomena at hand. Bolender's approach is entirely new to social science, although it is fundamental to physics and evident in biology as well. Recognizing these nested symmetry subgroups does not deny other sorts of patterns in human life, but provides new insights that in turn raise intriguing new questions. In particular, Bolender's analysis of symmetries and asymmetries in mental representations of social relations forces us to consider whether this explanation contradicts existing theories, and in any case whether it is a better explanation than existing theories. If it complements existing theories, we have to figure out how the theories fit together.

My view is that to fully understand any phenomenon-and certainly to understand social relations-we need to understand how human relationships are shaped by cultures, biology, minds, and physics. I believe that social relations are joint products of all of these processes. What Bolender does is to show that there is a truly fundamental self-organizing physical process underlying social relationships that has hitherto been ignored. And he links this fundamental physics to neuropsychology, hypothesizing that the structures of social relations are produced by a pattern generator in the brain. In any case, Bolender transcends the divide between structural and dynamic explanations: mental representations of social relations are structured by symmetries, which break in determinable order, yielding descending subsets of previous symmetries. This theory thus explains what is simple in social relations and it explains how-and in one sense whymore complex forms of social relations emerge.

In contrast to nearly all previous accounts of social relations, it is not a functional account. Indeed it offers an account of the neurocognitive bases of sociality that in some respects aims to displace, and in other respects complement, adaptationist functionalism. Bolender's discussion of the respective roles of physics and natural selection in the genesis of mental representations of social relations is sophisticated, well informed, and challenging. It will make some functional-adaptive evolutionists squirm (it made me squirm), but it will make everyone think more deeply about what evolution explains, what it doesn't explain, and how the two fit together.

My own view is that natural selection "utilizes" the symmetries and symmetry-breakdowns of physics for their adaptive functions-but only when they are adaptively functional. From this point of view, the symmetries of limbs and gaits are products of natural selection working in a world whose physics determines that efficient but flexible locomotion requires symmetrical support and motion. In water, the physics is different; the most efficient but flexible locomotion there favors a different symmetry, that of a single oscillating tail. In several phyla, natural selection independently converged on oscillating tails for swimming, and in several other phyla converged on symmetrically gaited legs for walking. Some phyla have actually gone back and forth between the two realms: the remote ancestors of mammals swam, but some mammals re-entered the water and re-evolved swimming tails. Cetaceans eventually lost their external limbs. Neither physics alone nor natural selection alone can explain these symmetries, or their history, but the interaction of physics with natural selection does explain them.

What about the structures of social relations? Bolender takes a bold position about the implication of the symmetries of social relations and their chain of symmetry breaking, inferring that these patterns imply that social relations result from physics, not natural selection. I hope that future discussions building on Bolender's analysis will consider whether symmetry and symmetry breaking sequences in the structures of social relations could be constructed from experience, either during one lifetime or cumulatively over many lifetimes in conjunction with cultural transmission or through natural selection. That is, would it be possible for a person, a society, or a species to discover that these four particular structures "worked better" than others? If so, would natural selection, operating though a process of Baldwinian selection, assimilate this experience into the prior expectations built into the organism? Likewise, are these forms of coordination more stable or more efficient than others? Such functional advantages might result from the coherence of their symmetries.

Next page
Light

Font size:

Reset

Interval:

Bookmark:

Make

Similar books «The Self-Organizing Social Mind»

Look at similar books to The Self-Organizing Social Mind. We have selected literature similar in name and meaning in the hope of providing readers with more options to find new, interesting, not yet read works.


Reviews about «The Self-Organizing Social Mind»

Discussion, reviews of the book The Self-Organizing Social Mind and just readers' own opinions. Leave your comments, write what you think about the work, its meaning or the main characters. Specify what exactly you liked and what you didn't like, and why you think so.