• Complain

Joel Levy - Why We Do the Things We Do: Psychology in a Nutshell

Here you can read online Joel Levy - Why We Do the Things We Do: Psychology in a Nutshell full text of the book (entire story) in english for free. Download pdf and epub, get meaning, cover and reviews about this ebook. year: 2015, publisher: Michael OMara, genre: Romance novel. Description of the work, (preface) as well as reviews are available. Best literature library LitArk.com created for fans of good reading and offers a wide selection of genres:

Romance novel Science fiction Adventure Detective Science History Home and family Prose Art Politics Computer Non-fiction Religion Business Children Humor

Choose a favorite category and find really read worthwhile books. Enjoy immersion in the world of imagination, feel the emotions of the characters or learn something new for yourself, make an fascinating discovery.

Joel Levy Why We Do the Things We Do: Psychology in a Nutshell
  • Book:
    Why We Do the Things We Do: Psychology in a Nutshell
  • Author:
  • Publisher:
    Michael OMara
  • Genre:
  • Year:
    2015
  • Rating:
    5 / 5
  • Favourites:
    Add to favourites
  • Your mark:
    • 100
    • 1
    • 2
    • 3
    • 4
    • 5

Why We Do the Things We Do: Psychology in a Nutshell: summary, description and annotation

We offer to read an annotation, description, summary or preface (depends on what the author of the book "Why We Do the Things We Do: Psychology in a Nutshell" wrote himself). If you haven't found the necessary information about the book — write in the comments, we will try to find it.

Can you really tell a criminal by the bumps on his head? What does a memory look like? Can a machine think? Why are some people shy? Is it better not to feel so much?
These are some of the many questions that have troubled the minds of some of the greatest thinkers in human civilization and are discussed in this comprehensive yet accessible introduction to psychology.
The complex workings of the mind have fascinated mankind for centuries, but the key theories of psychology are often so complicated that it is almost impossible for the casual reader to understand. In Why We Do the Things We Do, Joel Levy unlocks the important studies and theories in a series of simple questions and answers that shine new and uncomplicated light on the important aspects of psychology. This book will demystify the key questions by tracking their origins in the writings of some of the most prominent thinkers in various fields, showing how these ideas and concepts have developed over time.
With each section broken down into the key concepts, issues and arguments, considering how these ideas influence the way we all go about our daily lives, Why We Do the Things We Do will illuminate this fascinating subject.

Joel Levy: author's other books


Who wrote Why We Do the Things We Do: Psychology in a Nutshell? Find out the surname, the name of the author of the book and a list of all author's works by series.

Why We Do the Things We Do: Psychology in a Nutshell — read online for free the complete book (whole text) full work

Below is the text of the book, divided by pages. System saving the place of the last page read, allows you to conveniently read the book "Why We Do the Things We Do: Psychology in a Nutshell" online for free, without having to search again every time where you left off. Put a bookmark, and you can go to the page where you finished reading at any time.

Light

Font size:

Reset

Interval:

Bookmark:

Make

By the same author:

Freudian Slips

Why? Answers to Everyday Scientific Questions

To Mike and Christina who helped make this book possible First published in - photo 1

To Mike and Christina,

who helped make this book possible.

First published in Great Britain in 2015 by

Michael OMara Books Limited

9 Lion Yard

Tremadoc Road

London SW4 7NQ

Copyright Michael OMara Books Limited 2015

All rights reserved. You may not copy, store, distribute, transmit, reproduce or otherwise make available this publication (or any part of it) in any form, or by any means (electronic, digital, optical, mechanical, photocopying, recording or otherwise), without the prior written permission of the publisher. Any person who does any unauthorized act in relation to this publication may be liable to criminal prosecution and civil claims for damages.

A CIP catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library.

ISBN: 978-1-78243-412-2 in hardback print format

ISBN: 978-1-78243-410-8 in ebook format

Designed and typeset by K.DESIGN, Winscombe, Somerset

www.mombooks.com

Contents

Psychology is the study of the mind. The word itself comes from the Greek root psyche, meaning the mind or soul, and the suffix ology, from the Greek logos, meaning the study of. The term psychology was not coined until around the sixteenth century and only came into popular use in the eighteenth century. In fact, psychology was not practised as an explicitly identified discipline until the late nineteenth century. Its roots, however, can be traced back much further than this.

Folk psychology

All humans are what might be termed folk or nave psychologists, in that everyone considers, interprets and predicts their own and other peoples thoughts and behaviour. The ability to surmise what might be going ). So our everyday psychologizing could be said to lie at the root of what makes us and has made us human.

Body and brain

As a practice in the academic or professional sense, precursors of psychology can be identified in pre-modern and alternative models of the psyche and its relation to the world. In Ancient Greece, for instance, a form of dream therapy was practised 2,500 years before Jung (see for more on this).

Ancient Greek medicine reflected aspects of what today is known as a holistic approach, recognizing the role of the psyche in determining bodily health. Medieval and Early Modern medicine developed from Classical antecedents and reflected this psychological element in theories such as the four humours blood, phlegm, black bile and yellow bile that were said to govern mood and character. Mental illness was likewise primarily viewed as an expression of physiological imbalances.

Contrary to popular belief demonic possession was not the most common cause to - photo 2

Contrary to popular belief, demonic possession was not the most common cause to which insanity was attributed. Evidence from records of medieval inquisitions (that is, inquests to determine sanity, aka idiocy) show that there was generally a clear view of madness as having physical, bodily causes, with almost no appeal to supernatural agency. Madness was overwhelmingly perceived as a disorder of the body and brain, points out medieval historian David Roffe (Perceptions of insanity in medieval England, 1998). Where possible, specific causes were attributed for example, in 1309 Bartholemew de Sakeville was said to have become an idiot after developing an acute fever, while in 1349 Robert de Irthlingborough was found to have lost his memory and gone insane after he had been struck on the head by a lance while jousting.

Although this model of the physiological basis of psychology still informed psychiatric thinking and practice into the eighteenth century, humoral theory had begun to fall out of favour from the Renaissance onward. Increased enthusiasm for anatomical dissection, starting with Andreas Vesalius in the sixteenth century, found illness increasingly localized in specific sites of the body, so that lesions or disruptions of specific tissues replaced humoral imbalance as the cause of disease. At the same time there was an increasing emphasis on emotional states as a cause of mental illness in themselves: for example, grief as a cause of melancholy, or terror as a cause of hysteria. Yet even today, terms such as phlegmatic or sanguine are still used to characterize temperament and personality, demonstrating the long reach of the theory of the four humours.

Thus psychology was linked to physiology. In the sixteenth and seventeenth centuries, works such as Montaignes Essays (1580), Robert Burtons The Anatomy of Melancholy (1621) and Shakespeares plays (15901613) saw an increasing emphasis on the inner life of the mind, but there was little separation between psychology and philosophy.

A scientific approach

Our modern conception of psychology arose from movements such as mesmerism and phrenology (see ). Though now recognized as pseudosciences, these approaches did much to advance and legitimize medical and scientific study of the mind. The genesis of psychological science can be traced back to the laboratories of Wilhelm Wundt, who in 1879 opened the Institute for Experimental Psychology at the University of Leipzig in Germany. Wundts background was in medicine, and he was determined to put psychology on a scientific footing, having learnt what is now called psychophysics from his teacher Hermann von Helmholtz. Psychophysics is the study of the response of the nervous system to physical stimuli. Helmholtz investigated topics such as the levels at which luminosity of light or loudness of sound become perceptible, and the speed of transmission of electrical impulses in nerves.

).

James and functionalism

Around the same time as Wundt was working in Germany, the physician, philosopher and psychologist William James was also developing psychology in the United States. His approach stressed the functions (which is to say, purpose and utility) of behaviour and thoughts, and so his school came to be known as functionalism. Perhaps Jamess greatest contribution to psychology was his landmark 1890 textbook The Principles of Psychology, which set out themes of psychology that are still current today, such as brain function, consciousness and the self, perception, instinct, memory and emotion. Later, James somewhat disavowed psychology and his famous book, preferring philosophy, but the definition that he set out in the Principles remains one of the best known:

Psychology is the Science of Mental Life, both of its phenomena and of their conditions... The Phenomena are such things as we call feelings, desires, cognition, reasoning, decisions and the like.

Joel Levy

Picture 3

The psychology of prehistoric peoples is hard to study because it can only be inferred from archeological remains. In terms of psychiatry (the medical treatment of people with mental illness or disorders), the most striking evidence from prehistory comes from skulls that had been drilled with holes. This is known as trepanation or trephination, and the traditional interpretation is that it indicated widespread belief that mental illness was due to bad spirits, which could be released or exorcized by providing a physical outlet; hence the hole. It is equally if not more plausible, however, that trepanation was performed for straightforward and clinically appropriate reasons, such as relieving pressure caused by blows to the head, or removing splinters or blood clots from the same cause.

Next page
Light

Font size:

Reset

Interval:

Bookmark:

Make

Similar books «Why We Do the Things We Do: Psychology in a Nutshell»

Look at similar books to Why We Do the Things We Do: Psychology in a Nutshell. We have selected literature similar in name and meaning in the hope of providing readers with more options to find new, interesting, not yet read works.


Reviews about «Why We Do the Things We Do: Psychology in a Nutshell»

Discussion, reviews of the book Why We Do the Things We Do: Psychology in a Nutshell and just readers' own opinions. Leave your comments, write what you think about the work, its meaning or the main characters. Specify what exactly you liked and what you didn't like, and why you think so.